[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]
Re: mechanical cochlear model
At 8:59 PM -0500 3/9/10, David Mountain wrote:
... When this energy reaches the beginning of
the peak region, some process, usually called
the "cochlear amplifier" and involving outer
hair cells, takes over and shapes the peak
response.
David, I like your definition and description,
with one caveat: it is often said that the
"cochlear amplifier" is restricted to a narrow
region before the peak, or it's described as
"taking over", or as a different mechanism from
the traveling wave. That's OK, but I think it's
perhaps more useful to say that the mechanism
operates everywhere, and contributes to the
physics that defines the wave equation and its
dispersion relation.
The nonlinear properties of the mechanism that
adds energy to the traveling wave gives rise to
important nonlinear properties, including
compression and distortion products. But for the
analysis of the traveling wave, it is useful to
treat as linear, or approximately linearized at a
given operating level. Then its properties can
be described simply, in term of how the gain or
forces relate to frequency and wavelength. In
the region more basal than an octave before best
place, the wavelength is quite long, and the
energy moves through quite quickly, mostly away
from the membrane, so the effect of the cochlear
amplifier is necessarily very small. The effect
comes on gradually, and is greatest where the
wavelength is short and the energy is
concentrated near the membrane. The effect can
be described very simply as a
negative-resistance-like part of the membrane
impedance, in impedance-based models.
It's certainly true what you say, that we don't
yet understand the micromechanical mechanisms of
the cochlear amplifier, though we're a lot closer
than we were ten years ago. But there's ample
evidence that its effect can be fit into a
traveling wave model, and that such models do a
credible job of fitting both neural and
mechanical data simultaneously. To the extent
that there are remaining discrepancies, there are
lots of people paying explicit attention to
those, and trying to resolve them. For example,
if a high-frequency plateau is seen in mechanical
data, and not in neural data, what does that
mean, and how can it be resolved? The problem is
being addressed head-on, several ways, not being
ignored as some would suggest. It's a tiny
effect, in terms of BM motion and energy, but
understanding it is likely to help complete our
understanding of the whole system.
Generally speaking, we've seen a convergence of
experimental data, neural, mechanical, even
psychophysical, and physics-based models that
describe how it all fits together, over the last
40 years (measuring back to Rhode's 1971
mechanical measurements that showed the kind of
nonlinearity near the peak that was already well
known in neural data).
If people hadn't been questioning our
understanding, our data, and our interpretations
all along the way, we wouldn't be in the
relatively good situation we're in today. A few
years ago in a talk I criticized Helmholtz and
Ohm for their support of the phase-blind spectral
analysis point of view of cochlear function; I
realized later that I was off-base on this. Like
Bekesy and others that came since, they were
operating half-blind, with limited apparatus,
technique, and outlook, and doing their best in
good faith to advance the science. So I
sensitized myself to such criticism of our
forebears, which is why I sometimes speak up
sounding a bit annoyed when I see unwarranted
accusations like Martin's "Bekesy and his
followers succeeded in neglecting all unwelcome
evidence." The historical evidence shows
otherwise, and it's not a constructive
interpretation of where the field has been or is
headed, even if it can be shown to be true that
certain individuals and groups were temporarily
misled by adhering too long to a particular
conception when the data were trying to take them
elsewhere.
David, I count you and Jont and some others here
as among those who drove progress over the last
few decades, and who influenced my own progress
in understanding this system, through your work
and your generous help. And I'm sure you
understand that when I argue with you, it's part
of addressing the issues that the data and models
bring to our attention. I look forward to more
from you, Jont, Reinhart, and others, and hope
that those with alternative ideas to contribute
will be able to do so without falling into that
negativity thing that I fell into myself with
Helmholtz and Ohm.
Dick