Dear Boris,
Do you have any evidence of occurring? For the various journals where I contribute as Associate Editor, the role of the Editor is deciding a manuscript is minor, until there is a conflict, such as if the author disputes a rejection. They also validation the Associate Editors recommendation, as the Editor has the "final" say.
As Associate Editir, rejection without review must be justified, and is usually only permitted if the manuscript is judged out-of-school, which again can be refuted to the editor. I also know the latter is possible as I have been in such situation as the author and succeed in my rebuttal.
I have never seen not hear discussion of a case of blind rejection without reason or chance for rebuttal.
Of course, I'm open to hear others experience, but this seems like a boogeyman.
-- Brian FG Katz Equipe LAM : Lutheries Acoustique Musique Sorbonne Université, CNRS, Institut ∂'Alembert
-------- Original message -------- From: Boris Gourévitch <boris@xxxxxxxxx> Date: 6/7/23 06:07 (GMT+01:00) To: AUDITORY@xxxxxxxxxxxxxxx Subject: Re: [AUDITORY] [External] Re: [AUDITORY] arXiv web of trust
Dear all,
I think most of us might start to be a
bit exhausted with this discussion which is very interesting
though. I'll thus try to be short. I'm just surprised that nobody
mentioned (if I read correctly all the discussion) that editors
might be the biggest bias here, even more than reviewers. After
all, especially in the biggest journals, their role is more than
crucial: they might reject the paper even before peer review, with
little justification, or push for it to be reviewed and later
accepted. Yet, that's the first moment where the reputation of the
authors, the personal relationship of the editor with them, the
way in which results are presented or packaged, the methods used
and the personal view of the editor on the field can heavily bias
his judgment and determine the fate of the paper, beyond the
intrinsic qualities of the study. That's important because big
journals can make a career.
How many times did we think that a
paper more or less deserved to be in such a big journal
(objectively and without jealousy of course 😅) ? That's because
one person first judged the manuscript as deserving to be
reviewed, before later also weighing up possible mixed reviews as
a second step. This is even worse in journals publishing reviews
which might be invited or for which reviews are little used.
That's also why it's tempting to use preprints. No more
frustrations with this system. Anyway, from my point of view, the
editor work is less than transparent. Most of them likely do a
good job. But we don't even know.
Best wishes
Boris
Le 06/06/2023 à 10:57, Peter Harrison a
écrit :
Dear
all,
Several colleagues have mentioned how peer review is unduly
biased by the reputation of the authors/institutions. I agree
that this is an important problem, but it's only fair to
observe that it applies to preprints too. In a world where we
don't have time to read every preprint, many people will still
end up using imperfect proxies for deciding what to read, such
as the reputation of the authors/institutions. In the absence
of a journal's mark of approval, these imperfect proxies could
grow more influential, not less influential.
Best wishes
Peter
Dear Brian, all,
Thank you
for a very enriching discussion. I just wanted to counter
Brian’s last email, regarding the neutrality of peer
review. There is extensive evidence of “status bias” in
the peer-review system in studies comparing single-blind
vs double-blind reviews. E.g. Huber
et al.
(2022)
https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.2205779119 or
Blank (1991)
https://www.jstor.org/stable/2006906. No system (or
person) is free of bias, unfortunately. I think
recognizing that these biases exist and being aware of
them when we are reviewing manuscripts can only make us
better reviewers.
Best,
Helia.
|
Helia Relaño Iborra
|
Postdoc
|
Hearing
Systems Section
Department
of Health Technology
|
heliaib@xxxxxx
|
Ørsteds Plads
|
Building 352
|
2800 Kgs. Lyngby
|
www.dtu.dk/english
|
|
|
I feel obliged to reply to some
serious statements made in recent posts. While i think
there is little doubt that numerous bias elements
(privileges of various sorts" are present in career
evolutions, recruitment committees, promotions, be them
academic or corporate, I must return to the discussion to
the topic at hand, in the broad sense, of the importance
of peer-review.
As a regular reviewer in various
journals (and fields of acoustics) what is judged is the
work on the page, no more and no less. No free rides are
given to authors of high reputation (sometime more
scrutiny), nor penalties to young unknowns or
unrepresented countries (sometimes more flexibility is
given). If the arguement for publication is unpersuasive,
it is solely on the merit of the presentation of the work.
I say it this way because again it is only what is on the
page that is reviewed. The work itself may be of high
standards, but a work is reviewed by what is stated, not
what is intended. As an Associate Editor, the same is
true. Specific knowledge of the author is really only
needed to assure lack of direct conflicts of interest in
selecting reviewers. I have never considered the
background, academic or career history of an author in
accepting or rejecting a manuscript. I would even go so
far as to say if one considers these elements in one's
reviews they should probably recuse themselves from such
benevolent activities to the community.
Finally, returning to the question of
arXiv and preprints, where this all started, I don't think
anyone came out against them on the whole, but they should
be taken for what they are, and no more. They are a
scientific blog or a conference proceeding. They do not
hold the same value, or represent the same rigor of
critique, that a journal article has passed. Thie
difference is clear. However, it is only really relevant
in a few circumstances: as a substantive citation in
another journal article, in an academic/research career
application/review, or a project proposal (a version of
the previous point). If one doesn't require these
elements, and that is a choice, then one isn't limited by
the means one chooses to disseminate one's work. No one
has critiqued the use of arxiv and the like, per se, but
if one is competing on the quality of one's work, the
process of peer-review is the widely accepted passage for
some semblance of quality, for which no other alternative
currently exists. A review committee cannot be expected to
read every article, let alone the comments section, and be
required to form an opinion.
This does not say the process cannot
be improved, and that is also the motivation for journal
quality classifications and the exclusion of some journals
from being "acceptable" is those situations. Such rapid
publication and limited review journals are more akin to
arXiv than a reputable journal, though with fees, and
rightly so with regards to scientific scrutiny. One is
free to use them for what they are, but one should not
make claims that they are anything more.
At least, that is my perspective.
--
Equipe LAM : Lutheries Acoustique
Musique
Sorbonne Université, CNRS, Institut
∂'Alembert
--------
Original message --------
Date:
6/6/23 06:09 (GMT+01:00)
Subject:
Re: [AUDITORY] [External] Re: [AUDITORY] arXiv web of
trust
Hi Colleagues
I’ve been watching from
the wings on this discussion as I think our field is
in a real point of flux with respect to scientific
publishing and communication, and I don’t think I know
what’s best any more. Its been fun to watch a very
healthy and vigorous conversation unfold amonst my
esteemed colleagues – both junior and senior – and
I’ve learned a lot.
However, Matt (and
Deniz) made a very powerful point, that I felt the
need to weigh in on. They argue that the very nature
of scientific communication is pervaded by issues
power, positionality and discrimination. I don’t think
I realized this till recently (perhaps I was an Eagle
in that cartoon), but they are right. It’s important.
Les, I respect your
point of view. We should be having these open and
objective conversations and we should strive for
that. But we also have to recognize that this is an
aspirational point of view. In my view, the rhetoric
of science is not objective. Its persuasive. A
scientific discovery from my lab is not a fact until I
convince the scientific community to believe it (or at
least convince Reviewers 1,2 and 3). The rules of
science – statistical and methodological norms, peer
review, and the like -- are really designed to ensure
that this persuasion is all geared to some mutually
acceptable norms of objectivity. It often works and
there’s not much better.
But fundamentally this
is still a persuasive enterprise (as it should be).
And fundamentally, some people – by virtue of their
station and background – are going to be in a better
place to persuade their colleagues than others. We
commonly associate these issues of discrimination and
positionality with things like race, religion and
gender. And indeed these things matter – just look at
the disparities among the medalists of the ASA and you
can see for yourself.
But a good friend of
mine recently showed me how these kind of factors
extend all throughout academia. Are some fields
privileged? Are hearing scientists more likely to
discount a finding from a linguist or a social
scientist than someone who is solidly situated in
hearing science? What about a finding from a small
clinical population (a “niche” field) or an obscure
auditory phenomena vs. as opposed to a finding based
on the core “modal” NH adult in a sound proof booth?
Are we more likely to take a finding seriously if it
was generated by one of the top universities (in our
field) than a second tier state university? Or from a
new scholar that was trained by one of the best vs. an
emerging scholar who came to the field more
independently? What about a person who is changing
fields – migrating, for example, from a field like
cognitive science to audiology or hearing science?
What about clinical credentialing? Does that help or
harm our cases?
All of these things have
nothing to do with the objective argument that is
being made and the quality of the data used to support
it. But we all must admit that they do change how
much credence we are likely to give a discussion or a
paper (and each of us may weigh these differently).
Sometimes these are useful heuristics – if the methods
aren’t clear, but you know how a person was trained,
it may be easier to trust that the experiments were
done right. But sometimes this is just downright
discriminatory, like when we discount contributions
from outside what we perceive as the core field.
But how does this impact
scientific publishing?
Matt makes the valuable
point that as our field opens up to new viewpoints and
new participants, the view from those people may be
very different than the view from the people at the
top. We should listen. People do struggle to gain
entry to this field. I certainly did when I began
working in hearing science, despite my training at a
very good cognitive science program.
Peer review is part of
the problem. It can amplify these biases. And peer
review is not designed to “help” new entries – its is
designed to help a journal editor decide what to do
with a paper. So it often serves as an impersonal
barrier to entry. OF course, we cannot dispense with
it. But we should be actively exploring other
models. if this new generation of talented,
thoughtful, diverse and enterprising young scholars
wants to engage in novel modes of scientific
communication, I’m happy to listen and to contribute
to these new models.
theBob
On 5/31/2023 2:15
PM, Matthew Winn wrote:
***
Attention: This is an external email. Use
caution responding, opening attachments or
clicking on links. ***
There are
statements in this thread that cannot go
unchallenged, because they condone and
perpetuate harmful ideas that need to end.
Specifically:
1) “If one is not a sufficiently confident
and independent thinker such that one can
express ideas, arguments, disagreements,
etc. with anyone in the field, regardless of
stature, then that is a weakness”
This statement ignores the multiple power
structures that affect the lives and
employment of those below the ‘upper
echelon’ in the field. Expressing an idea
involves risk when your position is
precarious. Adapting to and weighing that
risk is a key survival strategy, not a
weakness. I have a blind spot for this risk
– not because I’m so great at science, but
because my culture gives me unearned respect
because of my demographics. For people like
me (and, I will note, virtually everyone on
this thread), we live in a culture that
insulates us from any sense that our voice
doesn’t belong.
I
could not disagree more. The suggestion that,
within our field, different cultural backgrounds
confer more or less ability to have productive
scientific discussions with anyone, regardless of
status is, as I see it, just plain nonsense.
Expressing an idea involves risk? Really, in our
field of auditory science? I can give plenty of
counterexamples to such an assertion.
2) “think about how such researchers earned
such status. It was not because they had
friends, it was not because people liked
them. It was because they established a
track-record of contributions that the
field, in general, held in very high
regard.”
This is a self-serving narrative that
reflects survivorship bias and which ignores
everything we know about how people act in
real life. Science is done by humans, who
have personal interests, biases, and who
live within a culture where status is built
on many layers of privilege. Every decision
we make is filtered by these factors, which
allow some people (like me) to accumulate a
variety of advantages at every career stage,
simply because of how they look, who their
friends are, and where they grew up. They
are more likely to have papers accepted, to
be selected for podium presentations, to
have a job application reviewed, to be
interviewed, to be hired, to be selected as
editors and reviewers, to be elected to
positions of leadership, and to be given
favorable treatment in the workplace. To be
taken seriously. If we pretend that these
advantages are ALL due to the scientific
merit of one’s work, we are characterizing
scientists as some species entirely separate
from the rest of humanity.
Again,
theoretical, social drivel. Lloyd Jeffress, Dave
Green, Neal Viemester, Barbara Bohne, and on and
on.
3) “Stature does not count. Everyone should
be held to the very same standard”
We all agree that work should not be judged
on the basis of who wrote it. But
importantly, the influence of stature
doesn’t need to be explicitly suggested in
order to actually take place. Similar to the
last point, the idea of equal standards and
equal treatment is a convenient fiction that
allows people like me to feel superior
because I can attribute my success to my own
hard work and merit, even though many
factors that led to that success were
unearned.
Again,
your theoretical musing. Not the reality in
auditory science that I have seen.
=========== ==============
What does this have to do with preprints?
The point is to consider that others have a
different set of constraints, and that our
definitions of merit are tailored to suit
those who are already enjoying a wide
variety of privileges. Consider the forces
that lead authors to think that preprints
are useful, and also whether you are facing
the same expectations and constraints that
they are. Numerous people have pointed at
the apparent generational divide on this
issue - let's figure out why. Graduate
admissions and fellowship review
increasingly expect a publication record
that far exceeds anything that would have
been expected of the reviewers when they
were at that same career stage. For various
reasons, the timeline of publication is
increasingly long. Exacerbating this, it is
no longer enough to simply conduct a good
study; one must also curate a data
management and sharing plan that includes
open-access data and documented code. One
must learn and conduct the latest
statistical techniques that their advisors
never needed to learn, and sift through a
much broader set of literature that includes
a lot of garbage. To compete for stable
employment, younger scholars need an
internet presence and must learn to
incorporate inclusive language in their
writing, even if that were not part of their
training. They need to express how their
work contributes to the reduction of harm in
society, despite being advised by some of
the people who are doing the harm.
None
of this, much of which I find to be mere
unjustified assertion, is an argument for shifting
the weight of dissemination of work toward
non-refereed open access. By the way, when was it
the case that a solid knowledge of statistical
techniques was unnecessary? Hey, you don't have
to wire together analog equipment to generate your
signals!
Preprints are not a magical solution that
can eliminate the multiple barriers that I
described above. But they have tangible
value, and reflect adaptation to a changing
academic landscape, rather than reflecting
some loss of “standards” that are designed
to protect those already at the top, and
which were established under an entirely
different system of constraints.
Preprints help address the needs for 1)
visibility and 2) quicker feedback on your
work from a wider variety of scholars who
might not have been invited to review,
simply because they were not in the network
of the associate editor. These factors are
often yoked together; the channels that
spread awareness of a preprint (like
Twitter) might also be the same channels
that generate discussion that becomes useful
feedback. The tendency (or need) to use
these dissemination channels probably
reinforces the generational divide on this
thread. I assure you that the comments I've
received from people enthusiastic enough to
read a preprint have had meaningful
influence and value. And those comments can
come from a wider variety of people whose
opinions have been historically discounted.
Experienced reviewers will always have a
place in our scientific discourse, but to
discount the benefit and potential of
preprints is to be willfully detached from
our current reality.
I never said one should not use pre-prints for
whatever benefit they can confer.
--
Leslie R.
Bernstein, Ph.D. | Professor Emeritus
Depts.
of Neuroscience and Surgery
(Otolaryngology) | UConn School of
Medicine
263 Farmington Avenue, Farmington, CT
06030-3401
Office: 860.679.4622 | Fax:
860.679.2495
--
Matthew Winn, AuD,
PhD
Speech-Language-Hearing
Sciences
|